Page 95 - DENG504_LINGUISTICS
P. 95

Unit 7: Vowels and Its Phonetic Transcription



        Finally, SSBE has a third set of diphthongs, which are known as the centring diphthongs as they all  Notes
        have the mid central vowel schwa as the second element. These centring diphthongs developed
        historically before /r/, which was then lost following vowels in the ancestor of SSBE; they consequently
        appear mainly where there is an <r> in the spelling, although they have now been generalised to
        some other words, like idea.
        GA speakers have a diphthong in idea, but still pronounce the historical [ ] in near, square, force, cure
        and therefore lack centring diphthongs in these words.
                                                                   r
        12.  Centring diphthongs
                          SSBE       GA
             near         wc         ir
             square       ec         er
             force        ]c/]+      o+r
             cure         •c         • r
        7.3 Vowel Classification

        The labels outlined in the previous section are helpful, but may leave questions unresolved when
        used in comparisons between different languages or different accents of the same language. Thus,
        French [u+] in rouge is very close in quality to English [u+] in goose, but not identical; the French vowel
        is a little more peripheral, slightly higher and more back. Similarly, [o+] in rose for a GA speaker is
        slightly lower and more centralised than ‘the same’ vowel for a speaker of Scottish English. None of
        the descriptors introduced so far would allow us to make these distinctions clear, since in the systems
        of the languages or accents concerned, these pairs of vowels would quite appropriately be described
        as long, high, back and rounded, or long, high-mid, back and rounded respectively.
        Furthermore, a classification of this sort, based essentially on articulation, is arguably less appropriate
        for vowels than for consonants. In uttering a vowel, the important thing is to produce a particular sort
        of auditory impression, so that someone listening understands which vowel in the system you are
        aiming at; but it does not especially matter which articulatory strategies you use to convey that auditory
        impression. If you were asked to produce an [u+], but not allowed to round your lips, then with a certain
        amount of practice you could make at least something very similar; and yet it would not be a rounded
        vowel in the articulatory sense, although you would have modified the shape of your vocal tract to
        make it sound like one. This is not possible with most consonants, where the auditory impression
        depends on the particular articulators used, and how close they get, not just the overall shape of the
        vocal tract and the effect that has on a passing airstream. It is true that the whole oral tract is a continuum,
        but it is easier to see the places for consonants as definite ‘stopping off places’ along that continuum,
        helped by the fact that most consonants are obstruents, and we can feel what articulators are involved.
        One possible solution is to abandon an articulatory approach to vowel classification altogether, and
        turn instead to an analysis of the speech wave itself: but acoustic phonetics is beyond the scope of this
        book. In any case, it is true that most speakers of particular accents or even languages will produce
        certain vowels in an articulatorily similar fashion. For comparative purposes, what we need is an
        approach which allows vowel qualities to be expressed as relative rather than absolute values.
        We can achieve this comparative perspective by plotting vowels on a diagram rather than simply
        defining them in isolation. The diagram conventionally used for this purpose is known as the Vowel
        Quadrilateral, and is an idealised representation of the vowel space, roughly between palatal and
        velar, where vowels can be produced in the vocal tract. The left edge corresponds to the palatal area,
        and hence to front vowels, and the right edge to the velar area, and back vowels. The top line extends
        slightly further than the bottom one because there is physically more space along the roof of the
        mouth than along the base. Finally, the chart is conventionally divided into six sectors, allowing
        high, high-mid, low-mid and low vowels to be plotted, as well as front, central and back ones. There
        is no way of reading information on rounding directly from the vowel quadrilateral, so that vowels
        are typically plotted using an IPA symbol rather than a dot; it is essential to learn these IPA symbols
        to see which refer to rounded, and which to unrounded vowels. The SSBE and GA monophthongs


                                         LOVELY PROFESSIONAL UNIVERSITY                                        89
   90   91   92   93   94   95   96   97   98   99   100