Page 134 - DMGT548_GLOBAL_HRM
P. 134

Unit 7: Training and Development




          the  host countries,  and whether  there is any need to learn the languages to facilitate cross-  Notes
          cultural success (Black and Mendenhall 1991).
          The “quantity” of an individual’s previous experience involve not only all his past experience
          with the host culture, but also all his previous interactions with a culture similar to the host’s.
          The “quality” of  the individual’s previous experience refers to  the intensity  of his  cultural
          interaction with the host culture or a similar culture. Intensity is measured by the frequency and
          degree of involvement of the interaction.

          Degree  of Interaction  =  (Frequency of Interaction with  Local Nationals)  x (Importance  of
          Interactions) x (Nature of Interactions)
          The more frequent the trainee is expected to interact with the local nationals, the higher the
          intensity of  interaction. ‘Where  the interactions are numerous  and  significant,  interaction
          intensity is high. The nature of the interactions plays a part in determining its intensity too. In
          this connection, the literature on communication has supplied  reasons to believe that  novel,
          two-way,  unique, face-to-face, long-term, and informal cross-cultural  interactions would be
          more trying than the opposite (Jablin, Putnam, Roberts, Porter, 1987).
          Job  novelty, the  third important situational factor, is task-related.  The more novel the new
          assignment is, the greater the assistance needed to help produce effective behaviour. This means
          that more rigorous training is required. Stewart (1982) suggests that to estimate the degree of
          job novelty relative to a specific trainee, three job characteristics should be considered: (1) extent
          of similarity in job demands between previous jobs and the new; (2) extent of similarity in job
          constraints between previous jobs and the new; (3) extent of similarity in job autonomy and
          authority between previous jobs and the new.
          In view of the fact that maladjusted spouses and children can directly influence the expatriates’
          success or failure in the host  country, the novelty of the foreign culture and  the degree  of
          expected interaction with the culture ought to be assessed in much the same way as is done for
          the expatriates (Black and Stephens, 1989).
          Black and Mendenhall (1991) integrate the notions of culture novelty, interaction, job novelty
          and CCT rigor by reasoning that high culture novelty, interaction and job novelty make the
          process of attention, retention  and reproduction slower. It is necessary  to add rigor to  the
          training programs because  it can  capture attention  better,  deepen  retention and  facilitate
          reproduction proficiency. Correspondingly rigorous CCT methods are  therefore selected.  To
          add, it has been shown that job novelty is relatively easier to adjust to than culture novelty and
          a high degree of interaction with the host culture (Black and Stephens, 1989). And since it is
          possible to quantify the respective dimensions, CCT programs  can be  customised for  each
          trainee.
          This means that a trainee who faces a highly novel job will receive relatively more training that
          will increase his technical competence. Likewise, where the degree of interaction is high, learning
          in this area heeds to be enhanced by emphasising on training topics such as interpersonal skills
          and perception. Where the degree of culture novelty is high, topics, such as country studies need
          emphasis. Thus, the trainee receives more assistance to more effectively acquire and emit the
          kind  of behaviour that will contribute to  impressive cross-cultural performance (Black and
          Mendenhall, 1991).

          Black and Mendenhall (1991) do not distinguish between cognitive and affective engagement.
          However, it is noted that affectively engaging training methods (for example, sensitivity training)
          are  surely more rigorous than  the cognitively engaging ones (for example, area studies).  In
          comparison with cognitive, information-gathering programs, affective and immersion-oriented
          programs require more personal involvement of participants,  especially when the need for





                                           LOVELY PROFESSIONAL UNIVERSITY                                   129
   129   130   131   132   133   134   135   136   137   138   139